Rasga, Célia Maria Batalha SilvaQuelhas, Ana CristinaByrne, Ruth M. J.2016-10-152016-10-152016Cognitive Development, 40, 46-59. Doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.08.0070885-2014http://hdl.handle.net/10400.12/4977Dados suplementares associados com o artigo e epígrafe estão disponíveis em: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.08.007We report four experiments on children’s reasoning about intentions using a new change-of-intentions task, in which an observer witnesses an actor carrying out an action, e.g., Maryhears her brother Tom say he wants to switch on the TV to watch a cartoon DVD. Mary goesaway and the reason for the action changes, Tom’s grandmother tells Tom to switch on theTV to watch the news. The experiments examine reasoning about false beliefs, e.g., What willMary believe is the reason that Tom is switching on the TV?, and counterfactual reasoning,e.g., If Tom’s grandmother hadn’t asked Tom to switch on the TV to watch the news, whatwould have been the reason he was switching it on? Experiment 1 reveals three effects,first,children aged 6 years make more mistakes than those aged 8 years, second, they makemore mistakes in false belief than counterfactual reasoning, and third, they make moremistakes for a desire changed to an obligation, compared to an obligation changed to adesire. Experiment 1B shows that the effects also occur for children aged 7 years comparedto 9 years. Experiment 2 shows that the effects occur for unfamiliar make-believe content,and Experiment 3 shows that they occur in stories with a simpler structure. The implicationsfor understanding the cognitive processes underlying children’s reasoning about intentionsare discussed.engIntentionsReasonsFalse beliefsCounterfactualsReasoningChildren’s reasoning about other’s intentions: False-belief and counterfactual conditional inferencesjournal article10.1016/j.cogdev.2016.08.007